What is JacketFlap

  • JacketFlap connects you to the work of more than 200,000 authors, illustrators, publishers and other creators of books for Children and Young Adults. The site is updated daily with information about every book, author, illustrator, and publisher in the children's / young adult book industry. Members include published authors and illustrators, librarians, agents, editors, publicists, booksellers, publishers and fans.
    Join now (it's free).

Sort Blog Posts

Sort Posts by:

  • in
    from   

Suggest a Blog

Enter a Blog's Feed URL below and click Submit:

Most Commented Posts

In the past 7 days

Recent Posts

(tagged with 'illusions')

Recent Comments

Recently Viewed

JacketFlap Sponsors

Spread the word about books.
Put this Widget on your blog!
  • Powered by JacketFlap.com

Are you a book Publisher?
Learn about Widgets now!

Advertise on JacketFlap

MyJacketFlap Blogs

  • Login or Register for free to create your own customized page of blog posts from your favorite blogs. You can also add blogs by clicking the "Add to MyJacketFlap" links next to the blog name in each post.

Blog Posts by Tag

In the past 7 days

Blog Posts by Date

Click days in this calendar to see posts by day or month
new posts in all blogs
Viewing: Blog Posts Tagged with: illusions, Most Recent at Top [Help]
Results 1 - 2 of 2
1. Animals could help reveal why humans fall for illusions

By Laura Kelley and Jennifer Kelley


Visual illusions, such as the rabbit-duck (shown below) and café wall are fascinating because they remind us of the discrepancy between perception and reality. But our knowledge of such illusions has been largely limited to studying humans.

That is now changing. There is mounting evidence that other animals can fall prey to the same illusions. Understanding whether these illusions arise in different brains could help us understand how evolution shapes visual perception.

For neuroscientists and psychologists, illusions not only reveal how visual scenes are interpreted and mentally reconstructed, they also highlight constraints in our perception. They can take hundreds of different forms and can affect our perception of size, motion, colour, brightness, 3D form and much more.

Artists, architects and designers have used illusions for centuries to distort our perception. Some of the most common types of illusory percepts are those that affect the impression of size, length, or distance. For example, Ancient Greek architects designed columns for buildings so that they tapered and narrowed towards the top, creating the impression of a taller building when viewed from the ground. This type of illusion is called forced perspective, commonly used in ornamental gardens and stage design to make scenes appear larger or smaller.

As visual processing needs to be both rapid and generally accurate, the brain constantly uses shortcuts and makes assumptions about the world that can, in some cases, be misleading. For example, the brain uses assumptions and the visual information surrounding an object (such as light level and presence of shadows) to adjust the perception of colour accordingly.

Known as colour constancy, this perceptual process can be illustrated by the illusion of the coloured tiles. Both squares with asterisks are of the same colour, but the square on top of the cube in direct light appears brown whereas the square on the side in shadow appears orange, because the brain adjusts colour perception based on light conditions.

These illusions are the result of visual processes shaped by evolution. Using that process may have been once beneficial (or still is), but it also allows our brains to be tricked. If it happens to humans, then it might happen to other animals too. And, if animals are tricked by the same illusions, then perhaps revealing why a different evolutionary path leads to the same visual process might help us understand why evolution favours this development.

Duck-Rabbit_illusion

The idea that animal colouration might appear illusory was raised more than 100 years ago by American artist and naturalist Abbott Thayer and his son Gerald. Thayer was aware of the “optical tricks” used by artists and he argued that animal colouration could similarly create special effects, allowing animals with gaudy colouration to apparently become invisible.

In a recent review of animal illusions (and other sensory forms of manipulation), we found evidence in support of Thayer’s original ideas. Although the evidence is only recently emerging, it seems, like humans, animals can perceive and create a range of visual illusions.

Animals use visual signals (such as their colour patterns) for many purposes, including finding a mate and avoiding being eaten. Illusions can play a role in many of these scenarios.

Great bowerbirds could be the ultimate illusory artists. For example, their males construct forced perspective illusions to make them more attractive to mates. Similar to Greek architects, this illusion may affect the female’s perception of size.

Animals may also change their perceived size by changing their social surroundings. Female fiddler crabs prefer to mate with large-clawed males. When a male has two smaller clawed males on either side of him he is more attractive to a female (because he looks relatively larger) than if he was surrounded by two larger clawed males.

This effect is known as the Ebbinghaus illusion, and suggests that males may easily manipulate their perceived attractiveness by surrounding themselves with less attractive rivals. However, there is not yet any evidence that male fiddler crabs actively move to court near smaller males.

We still know very little about how non-human animals process visual information so the perceptual effects of many illusions remains untested. There is variation among species in terms of how illusions are perceived, highlighting that every species occupies its own unique perceptual world with different sets of rules and constraints. But the 19th Century physiologist Johannes Purkinje was onto something when he said: “Deceptions of the senses are the truths of perception.”

In the past 50 years, scientists have become aware that the sensory abilities of animals can be radically different from our own. Visual illusions (and those in the non-visual senses) are a crucial tool for determining what perceptual assumptions animals make about the world around them.

Laura Kelley is a research fellow at the University of Cambridge and Jennifer Kelley is a Research Associate at the University of Western Australia. They are the co-authors of the paper ‘Animal visual illusion and confusion: the importance of a perceptual perspective‘, published in the journal Behavioural Ecology.

Bringing together significant work on all aspects of the subject, Behavioral Ecology is broad-based and covers both empirical and theoretical approaches. Studies on the whole range of behaving organisms, including plants, invertebrates, vertebrates, and humans, are welcomed.

Subscribe to the OUPblog via email or RSS.
Subscribe to only earth and life sciences articles on the OUPblog via email or RSS.
Image credit: Duck-Rabbit illusion, by Jastrow, J. (1899). Public domain via Wikimedia Commons.<
The Conversation

This article was originally published on The Conversation.
Read the original article.

The post Animals could help reveal why humans fall for illusions appeared first on OUPblog.

0 Comments on Animals could help reveal why humans fall for illusions as of 7/19/2014 6:56:00 AM
Add a Comment
2. A Questioning Thought

An obsession is a terrible thing to waste. That’s why I indulge myself in it as often as I breathe during the day. Well, quite often anyway.

My mind tends to bombard me on a continual basis about so many things that sometimes the overload causes mental awareness shutdown. What is awareness shutdown?

That’s when a mental question smacks the consciousness so hard that all other considerations, including where the person happens to be located, falls away. The person is left grappling with the various aspects of answering the question. Only that wrestling match exists until a resolution comes forward.

Case in point: Why do people feel the need to run on freshly seeded lawn, leaving gouging footprints behind?

I asked myself that question as I returned this afternoon from the Laundry Room. Do such people fear that they’ll leave behind no other memorable marks on this Earth? Are they trying to copy the “Footprints in the Sand” poster that most people recognize? What is their motivation?

Doesn’t sound very important on the face of it, does it? To someone like me, though, it begs significant consideration, since my obsession is writing. And all things relate to the written word, as all writers, whether of prose or verse, know. Without knowing the character’s motivation, predicting future behavior becomes chancy at best, abysmal at worst. Besides, that simple seeming event could birth a great little story.

For example: Rennie looked back along the line of his footprints. Rennie’s lip curled up at the corner. Deep divots in the freshly seeded lawn showed his progress. He’d wait for his father’s reaction to this before planning his next bit of revenge.

The motivation behind the character’s behavior reveals something about both the inner workings of said character and about his relationship with his father. In other words, dual purpose sentences emerge from one act.

Here’s another stray looking for a home.

A few days ago I watched part of a Food Network show where pastry chefs competed to take the rank of greatest pastry chef alive, or some such title. The episode’s challenge of the day was to create a fantasy masterpiece, capable of flying (the final test of the creation,) and do it with only one major ingredient – sugar. They could use water and food dyes, as well.

For those who don’t know what they were making, it was sugar glass. Sugar glass is that marvelous concoction that looks like glass, colored or clear, that is used in the film and theater industry to simulate the real thing. It’s also used for break-away bottles for bar fights and glass windows that will have people thrown through them, etc. Costs very little to make, can be recycled easily, and doesn’t hurt anyone in the process.

While I watched a few minutes of the process on that show, I began asking myself questions. I know what it’s used for in show business. Anyone with a candy thermometer, plenty of sugar, and patience can make it with practice.

I asked myself about the illusory quality to all things in the everyday world. Given the state of art today around the world, Photoshop capabilities to crop, blend, restructure, etc. the photos we see, and what we’re shown on TV, how much of what we see is actually real? Do we now live in a world of total illusion? Can we trust our eyes with anything shown u

0 Comments on A Questioning Thought as of 1/1/1900
Add a Comment